How the Brain Decides What To Memorize - Jaaj.Club
Poll
What would you do if you detected an alien signal?


Events

07.09.2025 17:28
***

Started
from the publishing house Collection Jaaj.Club.

Write a science fiction story up to 1 author page and get a chance to be included in a collective collection and get reviewed by renowned authors.

Jury of the contest

Alexander Svistunov
Fantasy writer, member of the Union of Writers of Uzbekistan and the Council for Adventure and Fantastic Literature of the Union of Writers of Russia.

Katerina Popova
A modern writer working in the genre of mysticism, fantasy and adventure thriller. The author does not deprive her works of lightness, humor and self-irony.

Maria Kucherova
Poet and prose writer from Tashkent. The author works in the genres of mysticism, drama and thriller, creates a series of novels and novellas in a single fictional universe.

Konstantin Normaer
A writer working at the intersection of genres: from fantasy detective and steampunk to dark fantasy and mystical realism.

Yana Gros
Writer-prose writer, the main direction - grotesque, social satire, reaction to the processes that are happening today. Laureate and diploma winner of international competitions.

Jerome
Author of the "Lost Worlds" series, specializing in space fiction and time travel. Author of numerous science fiction stories.

Artyom Gorokhov
Artem Gorokhov
Writer-prose writer, author of novels and many works of small prose. The head of seminars of creative community of poets and prose writers.

Olga Sergeyeva
Author of the collection of fantastic stories "Signal". Master of science fiction and mysticism, exploring time, memory and the limits of human possibilities.

***
12.08.2025 18:44
***

On Sale!

Echo of Destruction is a new post-apocalyptic novel
Zoya Biryukova.

A post-catastrophe world, an ancient war between vampires and werewolves, and a ritual that will decide the fate of humanity.


Zoya Biryukova is a gamer and dark fantasy fan. Her love for the worlds of vampires and werewolves inspired her to create her own story about the post-apocalypse and ancient powers.

***
02.07.2025 20:55
***

Already on sale!

A new story from Katerina Popova in a mystical novel


Anybody Alive? - Katerina Popova read online

***

Comments

Очень эмоциональный рассказ и, в то же время, блестяще раскрыта научно-фантастическая идея повествования! Спасибо!
17.09.2025 Formica
К сожалению, японский язык не поддерживается в тексте публикаций, оставил только английкую оригинальную версию. Спасибо!
17.09.2025 Jaaj.Club
https://akitahaiku.com/2019/12/17/world-haiku-series-2019-12-haiku-by-andrey-shtyrkovsky/

Haiku by Andrey Shtyrkovsky



spring rain melodies

yellow plum in the window

tea ceremony



春雨のメロディー

窓辺の黄色いプラム

お茶会



bird cherry color

the nightingale solo rings

fusion unity



鳥の桜色

ナイチンゲールのソロの輪

融合の調和



these free-flying moths

my adorable horror

thrill of meeting you



これらの自由に飛んでいるガ

私のかわいい手に負えないもの

あなたに会うスリル



zigzag on a vase

i read the word syzygy

the book fell and crashed



花びんのジグザグ

私はsyzygy という言葉を読んだ

本が落ちて大きな音を立ててぶつかった



azure serene sea

inside the field of vision

flip-flops in the sand



空色の穏やかな海

視野の範囲内

砂地にピーチサンダル



sweet like ripe cherries

fragrance reflected in glass

traces of a kiss



熟したチェリーのように甘い

グラスに映る香り

キスの痕跡



twinkling lights afar

twenty-four hours on the watch

lonely avenue



遠くにきらめく光

見張りを24時間

人気の無い通り



lullaby waxwings

mellifluous aquiver

woke up in the wood



子守歌のレンジャク

甘美に揺れて

森で目覚めた



flash instant insight

moment of endless present

being and meaning



一瞬の即座の洞察

終わりのない現在の瞬間

存在と意義



ineffable silk

in the heart of haijin path

aurora blossom



言いようのないシルク

俳人の道の中心に

黎明の花



— Translated by Hidenori Hiruta
17.09.2025 shtyrkovsky
Интересный сборник рассказов. Все рассказы очень разнообразные и не похожи друг на друга. Читаются быстро и легко.
17.09.2025 frolyagg
Спасибо большое!
16.09.2025 al.garanina

How the Brain Decides What To Memorize

09.07.2024 Рубрика: Interesting
Автор: vassyap
Книга: 
12520 1 0 18 1641
Electrical pulsations in the resting brain mark memories for storage, which supports advice about the importance of rest.
How the Brain Decides What To Memorize
фото: bobrlife.by
Electrical ripples in the resting brain mark memories for storage, confirming advice about the importance of rest. Djorgy Buzsaky first started messing around with waves when he was in middle school.

In his childhood home in Hungary, he assembled a radio receiver, tuned it to various electromagnetic frequencies, and used the radio transmitter to communicate with strangers from the Faroe Islands to Jordan. Some of these conversations he remembers better than others, just as people remember only certain events from their past.

Now a professor of neuroscience at New York University, Buzsaki moved from radio waves to brain waves and wondered: how does the brain decide what to memorize? By studying the electrical circuits in the brain, Buzsaki seeks to understand how experiences are represented and stored as memories.

New research from Buzsaki's lab and others has shown that the brain marks impressions worth remembering by repeatedly sending out sudden and powerful high-frequency brain waves.

Known as "sharp wave pulsations," these waves, resulting from the firing of many thousands of neurons within milliseconds of each other, are "like fireworks in the brain," says Wannan Yang, a doctoral student in Buzsaki's lab who led the new work, which was published in the journal Science in March 2024. They are triggered when the mammals' brains are at rest - during breaks between tasks or during sleep.

It was already known that sharp wave ripples are involved in the consolidation of memories or their storage. The new study shows that they are also involved in their selection, indicating the importance of these waves in the process of long-term memory formation. This finding also provides a neurological rationale for why rest and sleep are important for information retention.

The brain at rest and awake seems to run different programs: "If you are constantly asleep, you will not have memories formed. If you are constantly awake, they won't form either. If you only run one algorithm, you will never learn anything," Buzsaki says. "You have to have breaks."

Brain rehearsal


Buzsaki will never forget the first time he detected a sharp wave ripple. It was 1981, and he was a postdoc at the University of Western Ontario, listening through a loudspeaker to the brain activity of rodents. In nine years of research, he had grown accustomed to the rhythmic, melodic vibrations that awake animals made as they explored their environment. He wasn't prepared for the sudden "bong" that came from the speaker when the rodents were asleep.

What's going on. The rodents' brains in the awake state generated electrical activity that fluctuated at a constant rate. However, when they were under anesthesia, their brains seemed to irregularly produce much faster impulses.

Fast waves had been observed by other researchers Cornelius Vanderwolf described irregular waves in 1969, and Nobel Prize-winning neuroscientist John O'Keefe coined the term "ripples" to describe them in the 1970s. But it wasn't until Buzsaki saw them firsthand that he became obsessed.

Over the next decade, he spent much of his time in the lab trying to characterize these electrical bursts. In the late 1980s, researchers discovered that it was possible to get neurons to make stronger connections related to learning and memory by artificially stimulating them to make rapid bursts. For Buzsaki, these bursts were very similar to his waves. In 1989, he first hypothesized that sharp wave ripples might be part of a mechanism for forming and anchoring memories in the brain.

01fjbuhbuhkkk5bj1.jpg
Photo: wired.com

"He had the idea that this was not some noise activity, but something that had to do with the brain," says Michael Zugaro, a neuroscientist at the College de France who worked as a postdoc in Buzsaki's lab in 2002. "It was a foretaste of future discoveries because very little was known at the time."

In the 1990s and early 2000s, researchers took advantage of increased computing power and new tools that allowed them to record the electrical activity of more than 100 neurons simultaneously to better characterize the sharp wave ripples.

Buzsaki and other scientists have found that these pulsations appear to replicate the activity of an animal's brain, such as when running through a maze, but the pulsations oscillate 10 to 20 times faster than the original signals. According to Hiroaki Norimoto, a professor of neuroscience at Nagoya University in Japan, one 2002 paper that "made sharp wave pulsations very famous" showed that these pulsations reactivate a sequence of neuronal activity.

In 2009 and 2010, two papers, including one led by Zugaro, showed that sharp wave pulsations are involved in anchoring memories that persist for long periods of time. When researchers suppressed or disrupted these pulsations, rats performed worse on memory tasks. More recent studies have shown that lengthening or creating more ripples improves the rats' memory.

It became clear that the pulsations were being played repeatedly to solidify the memory. "The brain rehearses," says Lila Davachi, a professor of psychology at Columbia University. "Even in waking moments, your brain continues to rehearse and replay the past."

Imagine that an experience is a "melody on the piano," says Daniel Bendor, a neuroscientist at University College London. A particular sequence of neurons is triggered to record the experience, much like a pianist striking a particular sequence of keys.

Then, during sleep, the hippocampus repeats this sequence - but faster and potentially hundreds or thousands of times. The frantic, sharp wave pulsations travel from the hippocampus, which is the brain's staging area for "episodic memories" of specific experiences, to the cortex, which is involved in storing long-term memory.

However, no one has been able to explain why the pulsations spread when the animal is awake and resting. "They must serve some other purpose," Bendor recalls. Scientists had many ideas. Some hypothesized that the pulsations during wakefulness helped with planning or decision-making. Others speculated that they somehow alter or redistribute memories.

Another idea proposed by several groups was that playback during wakefulness and playback during sleep are closely related and may be the mechanism by which the brain chooses which experiences to remember.

Memory tests


At New York University, a room with infrared lighting held boxes of resting and sleeping mice. In the next room were mazes handmade of plastic and duct tape. One by one, the mice were placed in the mazes. They ran through it wearing electrodes that recorded the activity of about 500 hippocampal neurons, and learned that for passing certain routes they would be rewarded with water.

01fjbuhbuhkkk5bj2.jpg
Photo: wired.com

While exploring the maze, the mice took short breaks to rest or tidy up. And after the tests were over, they were returned to their home cage for a nap. The researchers continued to record their brain activity while they slept.

Yang analyzed the data by determining which neurons triggered during different trials. She saw many variations: some neurons triggered during early trials, while others triggered during later trials. Sometimes they triggered at different rates. This suggests that the brain captures the animal's experiences differently during individual trials. Notably, some trials were followed by bursts of sharp wave pulsations, while others were not.

She then compared the brain activity recorded while the mice were traversing the maze with corresponding ripples that appeared later. The trials that repeated more frequently when the mice were resting were the same trials that repeated when they were asleep. And those trials that were not reproduced when the mice were awake were not reproduced during sleep either.

The research team concluded that resting pulsations may be the mechanism by which the brain prioritizes experiences for memorization.

"Perhaps the ripples during wakefulness are the memory tags that consolidate certain impressions for long-term storage," Yang says. "In contrast, those that are not marked are not reproduced during sleep and are forgotten."

In December 2023, a research team led by Bendor at University College London published findings in the journal Nature Communications that foreshadowed Yang and Buzsaki's findings. They, too, found that sharp wave pulsations occurring in mice in the awake and asleep states seem to label experiences for memorization. However, their analysis averaged the number of different experiences, an approach that was less accurate than Yang and Buzsaki's.

The key innovation of the NYU team was the inclusion of a time element in the analysis, which distinguishes similar memories from each other. The mice ran through the same mazes, but the researchers were able to distinguish between blocks of trials at the neuronal level, something that had never been achieved before.

Brain patterns mark "something closer to the event and less like general knowledge," says Loren Frank, a neuroscientist at the University of California, San Francisco, who was not involved in the study.

"They show that the brain may be creating some kind of time code to distinguish between different memories occurring in the same place," said Freya Olafsdottir, a neuroscientist at Radboud University who was not involved in the work.

Shantanu Jadhav, a neuroscientist at Brandeis University, praised the study. However, he hopes to see a continuation of the experiment that includes a behavioral test. Demonstrating that the animal forgets or remembers certain trial blocks would be "real evidence that this is a tagging mechanism."

The study leaves unanswered a burning question: Why is one experience chosen over another? The new work suggests how the brain marks certain experiences for memorization. But it can't say how the brain decides what's worth memorizing.

Sometimes what people remember seems random or unimportant, and certainly different from what they would choose if given a choice. It feels like the brain is prioritizing things based on "determined importance," Frank says. Since research shows that emotional or novel experiences are remembered better, it's possible that internal fluctuations in arousal or levels of neuromodulators such as dopamine or adrenaline and other chemicals that affect neurons ultimately choose the experience.

Jadhav supported this thought by saying: "The internal state of the body may influence an experience to be encoded and retained more effectively." But it's not known what makes one experience more likely to be retained than others. And in the case of Yang and Buzsaki's study, it's unclear why a mouse memorizes one experience better than another.

01fjbuhkkk5bj.jpg
Photo: wired.com

Buzsaki continues to study the role of sharp wave ripples in the hippocampus, although his team is also interested in the potential applications of these observations. For example, it's possible that scientists may be able to disrupt the ripples as part of a treatment for conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder, in which people remember certain experiences too vividly.

"There's an uncomplicated result here - erase the sharp waves and forget what you experienced," he said. But for now, Buzsaki will continue to monitor these powerful brainwaves to learn more about why people remember what they remember.

Sign up for our free weekly newsletter

Every week Jaaj.Club publishes many articles, stories and poems. Reading them all is a very difficult task. Subscribing to the newsletter will solve this problem: you will receive similar materials from the site on the selected topic for the last week by email.
Enter your Email
Хотите поднять публикацию в ТОП и разместить её на главной странице?

Новое устройство может поддерживать жизнь мозга без тела

Исследователи из Техаса разработали метод, позволяющий сохранять мозг живым и функционирующим в течение нескольких часов отдельно от тела, - действительно странный научный эксперимент. Читать далее »

Человеческий мозг способен производить аналоги психоделиков

Чтобы справиться с падающим показателем психического здоровья, нужны более совершенные инструменты для лечения мозга. Многие эксперты считают, что психоделики могут сыграть существенную роль в решении этой проблемы. Результаты некоторых исследований показывают, что человеческий организм способен естественным образом производить такие вещества. Читать далее »

Активируем мозг

Можно ли активировать мозговую деятельность? Есть много разных тестов, которые определяют как развит наш мозг, или какое полушарием мы в большей степени используем в повседневной жизни. Хотя, можно, выполняя несложные и не утомительные упражнения, улучшать нашу мозговую деятельность. Читать далее »

Что такое гиппокамп и зачем он нужен

Когда учёные открыли этот отдел мозга, они решили что его форма очень похожа на морского конька. Дословно с греческого гиппокамп так и переводится - изогнутый конь. Читать далее »

Обычный препарат для разжижения крови нейтрализует яд кобры

Препарат, который обычно назначают для разжижения крови, можно использовать в качестве дешёвого противоядия от яда кобры, как выяснила группа учёных из Австралии, Канады, Коста-Рики и Великобритании. Читать далее »

Нанороботы, созданные на основе ДНК мышей, представляют собой точный способ лечения рака

Группа исследователей из Каролинского института разработали нанороботов, которые убивают раковые клетки с помощью специализированного пускового механизма. Читать далее »

Комментарии

-Комментариев нет-